The Brain as a Prediction Machine: The Key to Consciousness?
Part 1: Expectations shape many key features of brain and behavior.
Posted January 1, 2022 | Reviewed by Vanessa Lancaster
KEY POINTS
- Psychology has focused on how the past causes our present behavior. But the future can cause the present too, via prediction and simulation.
- The brain constantly makes predictions. Consciousness enhances the brain’s ability to form predictive models of the environment and the self.
- Faulty top-down influence of expectation on perception shapes illusions, weird beliefs, anxiety, depression, psychosomatics, and more.
The future, particularly cognition about the future, has been very much a back-burner issue in psychology for more than a century. The canonical human being, Homo psychologicus, is a prisoner of the past and the present…What happens when the canonical human becomes Homo prospectus, and our ability to think about our futures becomes our defining ability? – Seligman, Railton, Baumeister and Sripada—Homo Prospectus1
Psychology generally, and the scientific study of cognition more specifically, have tended until quite recently to focus more on past-oriented cognition (memory) and present-oriented cognition (perception) and less on future-oriented/prospective cognition (expectation, anticipation).
This has been changing. Increasingly, there is interest in how the brain is oriented to, and perhaps even organized around, forming predictions. And there is interest in developing an evolutionary understanding of why anticipatory cognition is so crucial.

Source: vexworldwide | AdobeStock
Understanding the brain as a prediction machine may help explain consciousness.
The brain evolved as an adaptation enabling organisms to better perceive and control the environment and their own internal state. Consciousness further enhances this ability by modeling the environment and the self.2 The ability to make predictions, guided by prospective representations—“if-then” possibilities, greatly enhances this modeling ability. A system that can model the environment and itself well can form simulations of the future environment and its adaptations to that environment.
In attempting to resolve the seeming enigma of how the brain produces consciousness, 3 it’s important to first understand “What Actually Is a Thought? And How Is Information Physical?” Thoughts are physical representations or maps. The mind is a kind of map. The brain, and its functional product, the mind, evolved as a map of the body’s relation to its external environment.
More evolved brains, such as human ones, can integrate past sensory experiences to form representations of things that are not presently “out there”—predictive simulations.
Beliefs are a form of predictive modeling.4
When you’re a brain inside a dark skull, guessing what’s presently out there is a form of prediction.article continues after advertisementhttps://89813e0b30cc52a58261461b49246846.safeframe.googlesyndication.com/safeframe/1-0-38/html/container.html
Anil Seth, a neuroscientist at the forefront of the serious science of consciousness,5 explained and developed the concept of the brain as a prediction machine in his book Being You: A New Science of Consciousness.
Seth suggested that even our perceptions of what is presently out there are, in a sense, just predictions or simulations (what he refers to as “controlled hallucinations”). These are formed by a continual process of updating predictions or assumptions (“best guesses”) as to what the sensory data are perceiving.6
As Seth explained in his popular TED talk:
Perception—figuring out what’s there—has to be a process of informed guesswork in which the brain combines these sensory signals with its prior expectations or beliefs about the way the world is to form its best guess of what caused those signals. The brain doesn’t hear sound or see light. What we perceive is its best guess of what’s out there in the world.7
According to this theory, what we perceive is strongly shaped by top-down expectations / predictions, not just by bottom-up sensory input.
How do the brain’s thousands of representations/models produce a feeling of unitary consciousness?
In his book, A Thousand Brains,8 the entrepreneur, scientist, engineer, and inventor Jeff Hawkins elaborated on how the brain’s knowledge is stored as numerous models or maps of the world. Research by Hawkins’ group has focused on how the brain has thousands of complementary models of each object it perceives.
He calls this the Thousand Brains Theory of Intelligence. Each model operates as a reference frame, physically stored in a tiny cortical column.9 These complementary reference frames provide “what” and “where” information; acting all together, they provide information about how the different aspects of the object relate to each other.10article continues after advertisementhttps://89813e0b30cc52a58261461b49246846.safeframe.googlesyndication.com/safeframe/1-0-38/html/container.html
Prediction underlies much of the framework for this theory. A cortical column can build powerful predictive models. For example, moving your finger from the bottom of a cup to the top can predict the sensation regardless of how the cup is rotated with respect to the finger sensor.11
So how come we don’t feel like we’re composed of a thousand brains, each with their own independent little model of the world? How come our consciousness feels unified? This is known as the binding problem. Hawkins’ theory proposes that the columns work together through their connections with each other, some of which are long-range connections crisscrossing the entire neocortex.
Through a process akin to “voting,” the different simultaneous models established by different columns encoding perceptions from different reference frames reach a “consensus” best guess (an algorithmic inference) as to what the object is that is being perceived, based on prior learned information (e.g. “the only thing that is consistent with what we’re all perceiving right now, based on the input from all the different senses and from all our different reference frames, is a coffee cup.”).
According to the theory, this system of storing information in reference frames and building models/representations of the world applies not only to physical objects. It scales up at higher levels of complexity and abstraction to concepts like mathematics, politics, and art.
Understanding the central role of expectations in cognition is key to understanding many quirks of human functioning.
The fact that expectations fundamentally shape perceptions and beliefs explains many of the brain’s most successful features, as well as its many problematic bugs.
Seeing is believing. But our perceptions are sometimes wrong. This may lead us to form mistaken (and often, intransigent) beliefs. Equally, believing is seeing—that is, our perceptions are shaped by top-down expectations/assumptions about what we are seeing, hearing, etc. When our prior expectations are mistaken, they powerfully influence us to perceive things in mistaken ways.article continues after advertisementhttps://89813e0b30cc52a58261461b49246846.safeframe.googlesyndication.com/safeframe/1-0-38/html/container.html
This is the basis of optical illusions. Magicians skillfully exploit our perceptual expectations to entertain and amaze us, as do psychics—some of whom are charlatans, others are themselves true believers—victims of self-deception as much as they are responsible for deceiving others.
We, humans, are highly suggestible, which, for many, combines with an incomplete understanding of science and other complex subjects (and an overestimation of that understanding), leading to beliefs in paranormal phenomena, mystical experiences, and other weird beliefs (including conspiracies), confused as to what is real and what is not.
Mistaken expectations play a role in many mental disorders. Anxiety and depression, the most common mental disorders, are characterized by, and some would say partly caused by, faulty expectations of, or predictions about, the future—overestimation of threat or failure.12, 13
Expectations (combined with and shaped by setting/context) also play a major role in the very different ways in which individuals experience psychedelics.14
Furthermore, expectations and unconscious biases in processing bodily sensations play an important role in functional neurological disorders (conversion disorders),15 and in other psychosomatic disorders. And expectations most certainly play a central role in the ubiquitous phenomenon of placebo response.16
Finding purpose in a Godless world
Seligman et al. have noted17 that the idea that events in nature might be purpose-driven has generally been considered unscientific—teleology has effectively been banished by science. Teleology18 is the idea that design and purpose are inherent in nature. Teleological views of nature predominated in pre-scientific religious worldviews.
Even today, such notions persist in the minds of very many religiously / spiritually-minded people who imagine that a spiritual worldview is compatible with science. A common example is an assumption that one can accept biological evolution and yet believe it to be intended or guided toward some ultimate end. This assumption reveals a deep misunderstanding of evolution.article continues after advertisementhttps://89813e0b30cc52a58261461b49246846.safeframe.googlesyndication.com/safeframe/1-0-38/html/container.html
Both of the major competing twentieth-century theories in psychology—psychoanalytic and behavioral, steered clear of teleology, emphasizing past causation of present behavior and excluding the possibility that any aspect of the future could cause the present.
But, as Seligman et al. concluded:
A conceptual error seems to have animated the lack of interest in the future. Something genuinely suspect—a metaphysical teleology of causation backward in time, of the present by the future—was conflated with something not at all mysterious, namely, the idea that, in a world with minds, behavior can be guided by maps of possible futures as well as the traces of actual pasts. In this way, ‘near future possibilities’ can explain concrete behaviors in the here and now.19
Even though the universe does not have an inherent purpose, living organisms, which evolved spontaneously and unguided on at least this planet and quite probably in many other places in the universe, are purpose-driven. Even the simplest organisms are, by definition, goal-directed. A purposeless universe became infused with purpose, and for humans, imbued with meaning.20
References
1. Martin Seligman, Peter Railton, Roy Baumeister and Chandra Sripada, Homo Prospectus (New York: Oxford University Press, 2016), p.x.
2. Most organisms’ brains lack consciousness or have only very primitive forms of consciousness, by most definitions.
3. Much has been made of what is referred to as the hard problem of consciousness—how subjective, conscious experience can be explained by and reduced to physical processes. As the philosopher David Chalmers, originator of the term “hard problem,” put it: “How can we explain why there is something it is like to entertain a mental image, or to experience an emotion?…Why should physical processing give rise to a rich inner life at all?” David Chalmers, “Facing Up to the Problem of Consciousness,” Journal of Consciousness Studies 2, no. 3 (1995), 200-219.